|
CrosswordBecause this whole thing about crosswords goes on rather at length, you'll find several intersections on this crossword main page where you can either scroll straight on and miss something you might like or take a detour. The only purpose of the detours is to spread out your downloading time. In the outlines at right the detours are marked with an ellipsis.
A MONOGRAPH ON I've worked an average of just over one crossword puzzle a day from 1972 to 2018. During that time I've learned a thing or two about crosswords, and I've even gotten a few of my acquaintances hooked on them. I want to hook you. Plus which, since I first posted this essay about crosswords on this Web site I've engaged in a number of electronic conversations with a number of crossword constructors and other fans. Without meaning to seem to draw a too-rapid conclusion, I can say that constructors of crosswords are more interesting to talk to than, say, your average doctor or lawyer or corporate chief. In the twelve sections that follow I'll try to pass on to you some of what I've learned, both from my own experience and from that of others. This monograph is written for everyone from the total novice to the thoroughly experienced player and even the thoroughly experienced constructor. My hope is that in this article you will find some useful information, perhaps some slightly interesting information, and maybe even a dollop of entertaining information, no matter who you are. Both as you're reading this and as you're working them, the most
important thing to keep in mind is that crosswords are for fun and relaxation, and I hope
you don't take them any more seriously than I do. In fact, I prefer the phrase
"playing a puzzle" to "working a puzzle" because that's the attitude I
think you should have. II. Why to play, and why not A. Why to play crosswords One good reason I play crosswords is that they're fun and relaxing. They're a sort of hobby, a pleasant diversion. Another reason is that they can be mildly amusing or even enlightening. For example, in a New York Times puzzle that ran on March 12, 2003, by Myles Callum, the theme answer is a takeoff on a Groucho Marx quip, strung in perfect symmetry throughout the grid: TIMEFLIESLIKEAN Here's another funny one, especially if you ever spent more than five years living less than three miles (exactly five El stops) from Addison AT Clark Street. In this NYT puzzle of October 23, 2005, by David J. Kahn, I'll give the clues and ANSWERS together. A man goes for a walk and FINDS A BOTTLE ON A BEACH. When he pops the cork, a genie appears and says, "I SHALL GRANT YOU ONE WISH." The man says, "I want to see PEACE IN THE MIDDLE EAST." He then hands the genie A MAP OF THE AREA. The genie studies it for a while and finally says, "This is impossible. So I GRANT YOU ANOTHER WISH." The man says, "I always wanted to see THE CUBS WIN A WORLD SERIES." The genie replies, "LET ME SEE THAT MAP AGAIN." (Needless to say, this joke made a lot more sense before the endearing Cubbies finally won a World Series, after over a hundred years, in a down-to-the-wire showdown that went into extra innings, with a rain delay beforehand, against the Cleveland Indians on November 2, 2016. The Cubs came back from being down 1 to 3 by winning three straight.) Crosswords encompass everything from the easy to the mind-taxingly difficult. The sense of satisfaction from completing a really hard clue is one every crossword fan will understand. Yet another reason is that crosswords're educational. I can guarantee you that every single person who knows me wants to be on my team in Trivial Pursuit, and the reason is that I've collected thousands of factoids in my brain from answering what I estimate to be closing in on nearly a million and a half crossword clues by now. Of course, many of those million and a half answers are repeats. For example, as every New York Times crossword puzzle fan will tell you, there's a limit to how many times you need to learn that Woodie Guthrie's son is named Arlo. And of course many of those answers are not educational at all. For example, I don't learn anything by giving the answer DROP to the clue "Fall." But if only two percent of the clues I've answered have taught me anything new, that's still maybe 30,000 factoids, and that's way better than zero. And it would be a mistake to think of those factoids as being sterile and isolated. While it's true that in any given answer you can learn only a few facts, when you consider how many answers cover a particular topic over a long time, you can eventually put together quite a few facts about it. For example, I have answered perhaps fifty clues about Galileo over the years, and in the process I've learned a lot about him. I know when and where he was born and whom he worked for and what his accomplishments were and a fair amount about what happened to him throughout his life. I've learned about the Catholic Church's reaction to his confirmation of Copernicus's discovery that the Earth revolves about the sun. I even know his last name. Here's another example. I have learned that Henri Matisse was French, that he painted water media, that he was a leader of the Fauvist movement, that he did a piece titled "Le Bateau" (The Sailboat), that "Le Bateau" appeared at an exhibition at the Museum of Modern Art in 1961, that some MOMA curators hold Masters of Fine Arts degrees, and that Matisse's sailboat piece was hung upside-down at MOMA for 47 days. And, believe it or not, I learned all that from one puzzle, the New York Times Sunday of November 23, 2008, by David J. Kahn. That's a lot of information to cram into one crossword puzzle, and I'll bet it's more than you knew about Henri Matisse till just now. You're probably wondering how a Matisse piece could get hung in one of the foremost art galleries in the world for 47 days upside-down. Believe it or not, this puzzle contains a bonus that gives you the answer. If you read the gray-colored squares they spell out SAILBOAT, and if you read the circled squares they spell out REFLECTION. Matisse's "Le Bateau" depicts a sailboat and its reflection on the water, and the "words" SAILBOAT and REFLECTION in the answer grid are reflections of each other. This is a shining (ha ha) example of the degree to which crossword puzzles approach true art. In case you're further wondering about that Matisse piece, it's cut out of paper and here it is first upside-down then right-side up.
Update of November 23, 2009 (exactly one year after the crossword puzzle appeared): Many events or states or affairs that are merely coincidental are mistakenly referred to as ironic. It so happens I lived in an apartment on 75th Terrace, then in 1994 I moved, and then I moved again, this time to a house also on 75th Terrace. I mentioned this to someone at a party once, and someone else said, "Wow, I used to live on a 75th Terrace too." The first person then said, "That's, like, really ironic." No, it's not. It's not the least bit ironic. That's just coincidence. (You can see another unexpected example of mere coincidence here, and, ironically, it also involves street names. Kidding.) And it's just coincidence that this update is published exactly one year after the puzzle was published, not irony. Irony can involve coincidence, but it always involves more. Anyway, today it was brought to my attention that the left image above, the one I said was oriented wrong at MOMA, is itself oriented wrong. Now that's irony. See the difference? It should look like the image below, i.e., this is how "Le Bateau" was incorrectly hung in MOMA for 47 days, not the way I showed it above.
Sorry (and thanks to Kalvik for bringing this to my attention). Also, in case you're wondering, my wrong hanging lasted not 47 days but 351. (Also, to be honest, they're all pretty much the same to me. Is that ironic or what? I mean, what if that one squiggle of cloud or whatever it is had been shaped just slightly differently? Or how do we know Matisse didn't slip with the scissors, look at it, and say, "Oh well, they'll never know. They'll buy anything I do. Hell, they'll buy it sideways if I call it 'Le Papillon' or 'Des Ailes D'ange'"?) Well, anyway, possibly the best reason for playing crosswords is that they allow you to revel in the small pleasures of playing with the English language. English, like any language, can be a toy to play with, like a kite or a computer, and I think it's good to play with it. It seems to me that it's desirable to understand English, or whatever your language is, as well as possible, to strive always to get more fluent. And not just because increased fluency in one's language increases clarity of communication. It's also because it's in language that you think. Much of your cognition results from how you think thoughts in your head, and when you do that -- when you reason things out -- you're doing so using language. If language is the tool of mentation, it is a tool we should always strive to master more. Yet another reason is that, according to some sources, playing crosswords or performing other such mental exercises can retard the onset and progress of senile dementia and Alzheimer's Disease. B. Why not to play crosswords They are time-consuming. According to my crossword timer spreadsheet, I spend an average of about 6 days annually playing crosswords. III. The rules of crosswords . . . Crossword puzzles that appear in The New York Times follow certain rules, and I hereby propose to list some of them. Some of them are of little interest, but many will likely help you play better if you know what they are, such as knowing that answers are never used more than once. Some of what I call rules are really more like traditions that authors invariably observe (and that crossword editors enforce), such as that the capital of Italia is ROMA, not Rome. Take this detour to learn about the rules of crosswords, then return here. In addition to the other information provided in this monograph, some of which should prove useful in playing well, in this section I list some specific tips, tricks and hints that I've found helpful in achieving the goals of finishing at all and finishing as quickly as possible. Some of these suggestions will be obvious to the experienced player but not to the novice. Others will seem like overkill to the novice but might interest the experienced player. But first let me tell you why I think you should learn how to play well, by which I mean being both as accurate and fast as possible. The two reasons you should be accurate are obvious: First, if you filled in a wrong letter you deserve to feel less satisfaction for having finished. Second, and more important, presumably you got at least two answers wrong, which means you might have mis-educated yourself. The two reasons you should be fast are less obvious. One reason relates to the reason I gave above about why not to play crosswords, which is that they are time-consuming. Clearly, if you can whip out a Monday puzzle in ten minutes instead of fifteen, that's five extra minutes you can use to get off the pot and on with your day. The other reason to try to finish a puzzle fast is because it adds fun. Harder. Since the mid-90s or so, I've wanted the New York Times' crossword puzzles to be harder. As I said in a 1996 letter to Will Shortz, the editor of the puzzle, "I wish every day were Saturday." (NYT crosswords get harder throughout the week: Mondays are the easiest, Saturdays the hardest. Sundays are 96% bigger but tweaked to be about a Wednesday or Thursday level of difficulty.) Because I've been playing crosswords for so long, I've gotten to the point where I find little satisfaction in merely finishing the Monday and Tuesday puzzles and some of the Wednesdays unless the theme is good. Faster. Consequently, in 1997 I added some challenge by timing myself, and you might want to consider doing so as well. My best time so far, which was on a Monday, of course, is 4 minutes 48 seconds. Experts can finish a Monday in 2 minutes, but I so rarely finish in under 6 minutes that I despair of breaking the 5-minute barrier ever again. Anyway, it's good to time yourself, to see how well you can think not just right but fast. I use a clipboard to play the puzzles on, and I've wedged a stopwatch under the clip, so it's easy to time myself. I usually finish the Mondays and Tuesdays in the range of 7 to 9 minutes. My good times for the Saturdays are around 30 minutes although I've done a few in under 20. My average Sunday time is well under an hour, although if you normalize it from a 21 by 21 grid to a 15 by 15 it turns out to be 11% faster than the average Friday, and my fastest Sunday turns out to have been just under 11 minutes. Here's a table showing how much faster or slower I have performed so far on average compared to other selected days:
Your own times will vary, of course, but if you're like me what this means is that you should expect to hit the biggest wall going from Wednesday puzzles to Thursdays. Hang in there. Once you get to where you can finish the Fridays, it's almost all pure fun. The data for the tables above and below came from my times tracker spreadsheet.
*Normalized from a 21 X 21 to a 15 X 15. The Sunday puzzle is 96% bigger. Spreadsheet. In April of 1997 for this essay on crossword puzzles I wrote a small spreadsheet to track my daily times. You may download it -- for free, of course -- to track your own. All you do is enter your times and the spreadsheet automatically updates you, on a Monday through Sunday basis, with your three best times, your three worst, and your average, as well as the standard deviation of times for each day and some other statistics. I myself record the times originally on a piece of paper in my clipboard immediately after I finish each puzzle, and every so often I transcribe the data into the spreadsheet. I've tried to make the data-entry part as quick and painless and unscrewuppable as possible. To learn more about this spreadsheet, which is written in Excel 2000 and no doubt can be read by or converted to other formats, just let me know. Update of May 2005: The spreadsheet now also contains a sheet that allows you to chart your times, to see whether you're getting faster or slower. You can see and read a little about the chart of my times here. Anyway, I regard finishing fast as being almost as desirable as finishing at all. I think in formal speed-solving contests they let you leave cells blank or get letters wrong and then penalize you severely for it. I've heard that the penalty is this: For each wrong cell they whomp you once upside the head with a warm walleye, and for each blank cell they use an outright carp. Update of October 26, 1999 Here are the rules for scoring in the American Crossword Puzzle Tournament (ACPT) held and directed by Will Shortz each year in Stamford, Connecticut from its inception in 1978 till 2007, after which because it became so popular it moved to a larger hotel, in Brooklyn, New York:
So, for example, if it's a 15 X 15 puzzle with 70 answers and you get them all right in 12 minutes, that'd be 700 points for the 70 correct answers, plus 75 bonus points for finishing three minutes under the limit, plus 150 additional bonus points for getting the complete, correct solution, for a total of 925 points. (Just so you'll know, there are people who can solve a NYT crossword in two minutes.) However, if there's just one cell wrong or blank in that same 12 minutes your score would be 680 points plus 50 bonus points for finishing three minutes early, or only 730 points total. So, apparently it doesn't involve being struck with any species of fish whatsoever.
As I said, some of the tips that follow might seem like overkill to the novice, and it's probably those very tips whose purpose is to promote speed, not accuracy. I do hereby note a significant drawback to trying to play fast, which is that you can't take the time to wallow luxuriously and languidly in all the information to be found in your reference works. If you don't feel pressured to finish fast, you may spend a few extra seconds or minutes or weeks reading about something that has caught your interest's eye. Learning about the universe out there and in our heads, and the language we use to interpret them is, it seems to me, always desirable. Take this detour to learn how to play crosswords well, then return here. V. Relationships between clue and answer It is insufficient to think of the answer as being a definition for its clue or vice versa. Frequently one of those is the case, but there are other possibilities. What's true in every case is that the answer and its clue form a relationship. It's the relationship that you need to noodle out for each pair of answer and clue in order to complete the puzzle. And in order to solve that equation, i.e., in order to identify that relationship where ANSWER = Clue, it's helpful to know which relationships can exist in crosswords. If you get stuck on a seemingly easy clue-and-answer relationship, it's good to return to the advice above about keeping your mind open to all interpretations, and a good way to do that is to mentally scan through the possible relationships. A. Equivalence. The commonest crossword relationship is that of simple equivalence, i.e., the answer and its clue are two expressions of the same thing. For example, the clue is "Capital of the former Upper Volta" and the answer, as if you didn't know, is OUAGADOUGOU. B. Commonality. In this relationship, each of the collection of elements of the clue is an example of the answer. For example, the clue "Betta, tetra and opah" could properly be answered with FISH. (Also note that in this example the answer FISH would be appropriate even if the clue had read "Betta, tetra or opah," since "fish" refers to both plural and singular. And while I'm on this example, let me wedge in another tip, trick or hint: Remember that some present tense verbs use the same spelling for the past tense form, such as "put" and "read" and the dreaded "set.") The less common commonality is the mirror of the relationship described above, in which the clue is the single element and the answer is the collection of elements. To stick with the example above for a moment, if the clue were "Fish" it would not be a violation of the crossword puzzle rules if the answer were BETTA TETRA OR OPAH. If you're thinking that this answer is unlikely to appear in isolation in a real puzzle, you'd be right. But it certainly would not be out of character if it were a theme answer. C. Set membership. A pair of mirror relationships is that of set membership. Either the clue is a member of the answer set or the answer is a member of the clue set. An example of the first instance is the answer ANIMAL for the clue "Insect." An example of the second instance is the answer INSECT for the clue "Animal." These are extremely simple examples, invented more for clarity than for how likely they are to be used in a real puzzle. In fact, it would be unusual for the clue "Insect" to be given for the answer ANIMAL or vice versa. The point is that neither pair would violate the rules of New York Times crosswords, so they are examples of two common relationships for which you need to keep a mental eye peeled. D. Fill in the blank (FITB). This relationship hardly needs further explanation. The relationship is that the answer is that collection of characters that completes the clue. A self-referential example here would be the answer BLANK for the clue "Fill in the ___." A slightly more complicated and typographically invisible sort of FITB arises in the form "Suffix with lob or mob" (answer: STER) or "Prefix meaning catholic" (answer: OLI). E. Analogy. In this relationship the answer can be the solution to a logical analogy presented in the clue. For example the answer to the clue "Dog : Puppy :: Cat : ____" is KITTEN. In the special notation used by logicians and mathematicians, the single colon means "is to," and the double colon means "as." So, to take another example, "Area : map :: volume : ____" is read "Area is to map as volume is to What?" (Hint: globe.) The number of outright errors and seriously questionable answers in New York Times crosswords is remarkably low. In the one page you're reading at this moment I've probably made more errrors of one sort or another than in all the 3,652.5 daily New York Times crosswords published in the last ten years. Still, errors and questionable answers do crop up, and it's helpful to know that. When they do arise, the ideal result for you as the player is both to finish the puzzle according to the answer grid and to recognize the error. How to complain. In about 1996 I complained to Will Shortz, the editor of the New York Times puzzle, about the answer TIL as an acceptable substitute for "till," as in "until." Neither "til" nor even " 'til" is a legitimate word in any dictionary I had consulted (despite the prevalent use of the second version by all sorts of uneducated and otherwise educated people, as though it were a contraction of "until"), so I wrote asking what authority allowed the answer TIL. I cited a number of authoritative dictionaries that either do not list "til" at all or list it as an illiterate version of "till." I even cited a few authors on usage agreeing with me, one of whom was the famous New York Times editor Theodore M. Bernstein. In response to all that I received a post card in which Mr. Shortz pointed out, quite correctly, that exactly one of my references allowed as how " 'til" was regarded as a variant of "till." That's all it takes, and so I was set straight about what's allowable as an answer to a NY Times crossword. It is as it should be. I also note that "til" has never appeared as a legitimate answer since I wrote that letter, although it did appear in the January 23, 1998, puzzle in answer to the clue "Up to." But that's not exactly what the clue said. In fact it said, "Up to, informally" (emphasis mine). Yay for me. FYI, "til" actually is a perfectly OK if somewhat obscure word meaning the seed of the sesame plant. Anyway, because there are so few errors, it's satisfying in a puny sort of way to catch them. Take this detour to learn about NYT crossword errors I think I've caught, then return here. There is a difference of opinion whether it's OK to use reference works to solve puzzles. The people who think it's not OK explain their position with these three points:
In reply I say:
The term "reference" as used here has different meanings. We can think of them as lying on a scale from not-really-cheating-much-at-all on up to full-blast, no-doubt-about-it cheating. At the good end of the scale, I think it's positively desirable to use references such as dictionaries and atlases, for two reasons. One is that the more you use them the more efficient you get at using them, which is good because it saves time. I know France is on page 28 of my atlas, and I often open my unabridged dictionary to within a few pages of the word I want. I'm willing to pay the price of believing I cheated a little in order to save time not having to look up France in the table of contents. A better reason is that if you do go ahead and use your dictionaria and atli and so on, such as how to pretend-pluralize words in Latin, you're bound to learn something. As you're rummaging around looking for whatever five-letter word you need, you can't help stumbling right smack into facts you didn't initially care about, facts you might find useful in understanding the universe of your life. Moving supposedly further up the scale from good cheating to bad, I really don't see any significant difference between using paper references and using the Internet as a substitute. Learning to use the Internet efficiently to get information is desirable, and it can save a significant amount of time. And the Internet, with its hyperlinks to anywhere, makes it so much quicker to follow up on whatever piques your interest. Furthermore, the Internet has a lot more information than any collection of paper references you have, unless your name is Library of Congress. Yet nearer to full-on cheating is simply asking someone who knows the answer. I had no idea that in hockey there's a deke or that in baseball there's an A-Rod, but I knew my brother would, so I asked him and got my answers. I didn't learn how to use my references more efficiently (unless you count knowing my brother's telephone number, which I'll give you if you have any sports questions), but at least I learned the explanations (decoy, Alex Rodriguez). Perhaps even further along the cheating scale is using a reference, whether online or on paper, that simply shows you all the possibilities for a given pattern of known and unknown letters. If the clue is "Plenipotentiary" and the answer LEGATE turns out to fit your puzzle, and if you don't know what either word means, you have nominally finished the puzzle but you haven't learned anything. I'd rather spend a few extra seconds using a more informative reference and learning something than finishing faster but remaining ignorant. The extreme end of the cheating scale is simply to ask online. Several sites purport to answer general-knowledge questions, with varying degrees of accuracy but in some cases within a minute, so you could simply log in to one and type, "What German industrial city on the Ruhr River starts with ES and is five letters long?" (Hint: Essen.) I used ChaCha.com for this purpose hundreds of times till it suddenly got unreliable. Yes, I was cheating, and no, I don't feel bad about it. As I say, I think the "no-references" standard is arbitrary. I would not finish maybe a fifth of the puzzles if I eschewed my references. As I see it, you will surely learn a lot more if you do use them, and I truly do consider learning to be a really good reason to play crosswords, no matter how many people disagree with me, which is a lot. Not only do you learn about the relationship you looked up, you can also fortuitously learn a bunch of other stuff along the way. For instance, just this morning (yes, this very morning) I was looking up the word "cline" in a dictionary (Rhu2) when I noticed the headword on the other page was "cliticize," which at first I thought was the Japanese pronunciation of "criticize," which of course drew my attention, so I looked up "cliticize" and discovered its handy adjectival form, "clitic," the definition of which you might want to look up yourself, following which you will probably be a lot disappointed but a little educated. If you're an experienced solver (or constructor) you won't care much which references I use because you'll have gradually gathered your own collection, but if you're a novice you might care a little. Take this detour to learn about the reference works I've chosen, then return here. One of the funnest features of New York Times crossword puzzles is the theme answers. Not all of them have a theme, and they can't help seeming a bit pedestrian or repetitive every once in a great while, but if you've read this far then often you'll think they're remarkably clever and entertaining. Crossword constructors play with language at two levels. Take this detour to learn about some outstanding NYT crossword themes, then return here. IX. Try writing your own . . . If you would like to learn how to play crossword puzzles better, the single best method is to play them a lot. The most difficult method (not counting having to read this really long essay), but a surprisingly educational one, is to try to write your own. There are two benefits to trying to write your own crossword puzzle. One is that you will have a more detailed appreciation of how difficult it is to write one at all, not to mention how much more difficult it is to write a good one. The other benefit is that you will become a better, faster player. Once you have sat in the author's seat for a while, you can understand better how certain answers get the clues they do. You'll ask, "If I were the author of this puzzle and this is the answer I need to complete this part of the grid, what clue can I give?" Take this detour to learn how to write a crossword, then return here. One particular crossword has generated a lot of interest, the puzzle I declared is the hardest one ever published by The New York Times. Also, it contains what has been for me the most baffling clue-and-answer relationship ever. Take this detour to learn a lot more about the hardest NYT crossword, then return here.
XI. Miscellaneous thoughts . . . Take this detour to read a few more miscellaneous thoughts about crosswords, then return here. XII. Conclusion From playing crosswords I have enjoyed many thousands of small doses of pleasure. If you are an experienced player, you know what I mean. If you are a tyro, consider the benefits as I see them. First, I've learned a lot. Second, I always revel in playing with and presumably getting better at using my language, which is the tool, the translating mechanism, I use to think thoughts and to communicate with other humans. And third, I've spent all those hours relaxing. You too can enjoy all those small and large pleasures, meted out in small daily doses. If you are a complete novice you will likely find your first several crosswords to be impossibly difficult; you simply will not be able to finish no matter how long you try. And that's OK; it's to be expected. But as you gain experience you will start to catch on. Do hang in there. You'll find after a couple weeks or months -- depending on how good you are with language to begin with and how many facts you already know and how good you are with references -- that you can finish some of the Monday New York Times puzzles. I can almost guarantee you you'll remember the first time you finish one by yourself. After a while longer you'll be finishing the Mondays most of the time and some of the Tuesdays. Within six months or a year you'll be looking forward to the Sundays, because you'll enjoy how the big Sunday puzzles give the authors room to develop their themes more deeply, and by then you will have caught fully on to what to expect from a NY Times crossword. You'll be crashing through those clues in no time, writing or typing in lots of them as fast as you can write or type, slowing down only to ponder over the tough ones, and taking in all the pleasure of the Cute Clues. And maybe a year after that you'll actually be looking forward to the puzzles that are -- pound for pound -- the toughest of all, the fearsome and unpredictable Saturdays. At that point you will have become permanently hooked. And in the meantime you should not hesitate to see whether anyone you know plays crosswords or wants to. Crosswords can definitely be a group activity, whether it's a group of ten co-workers in an office e-mailing or texting one another about the clues or a group of two people holding the puzzle between them and relaxing together in intimate contact. I'm not sure I went down all the detours . . . Take me back to the top crossword outline again. |
Outline of
I.
Introduction
Click ? at the top of any page to see all the nifty navigation aids, including Search Site and Site Map.
|
||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Outline of I.
Introduction Crossword links
|
|
|
|
|
|
|